How The Brain Sees Colour

Colour vision is only present in daylight or bright light (photopic vision), and is absent at night or dark vision (scotopic vision) where rods function. There are approximately 7 million cones and 120 million rods in the human retina. The two types of cells are not uniformly distributed. The central retina surrounding the fixation point, the fovea, is essentially rod-free, and purely occupied by cones. Rods have their maximum density slightly peripheral to the fovea, and both types of cell diminish in number towards the retinal periphery3.

The visual spectrum is made up of many wavelengths. The perceived hue or colour is related to the wavelength of light . Short wavelengths are seen as blue or violet, medium wavelengths are perceived as green and yellow, while long wavelengths are seen as orange or red. The primary colours of light are red, green and blue. Mixtures of these colours in various combinations create different wavelengths, and stimulate the cones in varying degrees.

Normal colour vision is trichromatic. Most of the spectral hues can be matched by an additive mixture of three primary colours taken from the long-wave ( red), medium-wave (green), and short-wave (blue) parts of the visual spectrum. Although the three primary colours are referred to as red, green and blue, the wavelengths of peak sensitivity do not correspond exactly with these colour names. The three cone types have overlapping spectral sensitivity which have maximum sensitivity in these spectral regions3.

In a normal trichromat, three wavelengths are required to match a given reference wavelength. This is because all the hues that the eye can see can be matched by different amounts of the three primary colours in various combinations. So to match a certain reference wavelength, a combination of all three primary wavelengths must be present3.

Dichromacy occurs when there are only two cones functioning. This means that only two types of cones can provide colour information to the brain, so only two test stimuli are needed to match a reference point.

Monochromats and achromats only need one wavelength to match the reference colour. This is because they can only match with the brightness of an object, rather than the colour of the object.


1 Foster, D. H. (ed.) (1991). Inherited and Acquired Colour Vision Deficiencies: FUndamental Aspects and Clinical Studies.
Macmillan Press: London.

This page hosted by GeoCities Get your own Free Home Page


1