Developmental Psychology© 1999 by the American Psychological Association March 1999 Vol. 35, No. 2, 445-459 For personal use only--not for distribution. Short-Term and Long-Term Effects of Early Parental Employment on Children of the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth Elizabeth Harvey Department of Psychology University of Connecticut ABSTRACT This study examined the effects of early parental employment on children in the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth. Minimal effects on children's later functioning were found. Early maternal employment status and the timing and continuity of early maternal employment were not consistently related to children's development. Working more hours was associated with slightly lower cognitive development through age 9 and slightly lower academic achievement scores before age 7 but had no significant relation to children's behavior problems, compliance, or self-esteem. Early parental employment appeared to be somewhat more beneficial for single mothers and lower income families. There was some support for the hypothesis that early parental employment positively affects children's development by increasing family income. ------------------------------------------------------------------------ I thank Conrad Schwarz and Len Milling for their comments on earlier drafts of this article. Correspondence may be addressed to Elizabeth Harvey, Department of Psychology, University of Massachusetts at Amherst, Tobin Hall, Amherst, Massachusetts, 01003-7710. Electronic mail may be sent to eharvey@psych.umass.edu Received: June 30, 1997 Revised: June 6, 1998 Accepted: June 6, 1998 ------------------------------------------------------------------------ The past several decades have seen an increase in the number of employed mothers, with a particularly large increase in the frequency with which mothers of young children are employed ( Bureau of the Census, 1994 ). Researchers who have reviewed the literature on the effects of maternal employment on children's development have agreed that there is little evidence of negative effects when children are older ( Belsky, 1988 , 1990 ; Hoffman, 1961 , 1974 , 1989 ). However, there remains debate about the effects of early maternal employment on children. After reviewing the literature, Belsky (1988) concluded that maternal employment during infancy had ill effects on children's well-being. Specifically, he concluded that infants who were in nonmaternal care for more than 20 hr per week were at elevated risk for being insecurely attached at age 1 and were more disobedient and aggressive between ages 3 and 8. However, his conclusions have been criticized on several counts. For example, it has been argued that the studies Belsky reviewed failed to take into account background variables that may have been confounded with maternal employment and children's well-being (e.g., Clarke-Stewart, 1988 , 1989 ). In addition, Clarke-Stewart noted that many of these studies were based on nonrepresentative samples. Finally, she argued that the measures of attachment used in these studies may not have predictive validity for children of working mothers and that more longitudinal studies are needed to determine effects on children's later functioning. In recent years, several studies have addressed some of these criticisms using the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth (NLSY). The NLSY is a survey of women who have been interviewed annually since 1979 when they were 14 to 22 years old. Beginning in 1986, children of these women were also assessed. Six published studies have used this sample to examine the longitudinal effects of early maternal employment on children's development, controlling for various family background variables. The results of these studies have been surprisingly mixed considering they used the same data set. The sample size and longitudinal design of the NLSY make it potentially valuable for illuminating the effects of early maternal employment. However, rather than shedding light on this issue, conflicting results of studies based on these data have added further confusion. Although these six studies all used the same data set, they each used quite different methodological approaches with respect to sample selection, construction of independent and dependent variables, and selection of control variables. The present study sought to resolve some of these differences through a reanalysis using an updated version of the NLSY that contained a much larger and more representative sample. In this article, I examined the six studies, explored how their methodological differences might have yielded discrepant results, evaluated their methodological strengths and limitations, and reanalyzed the effects of early parental employment by drawing on the strengths and addressing the limitations of these six previous studies. A Comparison of Methodological Approaches and Findings 1 Of the six previous studies, none used the exact same sample. Slightly different age ranges were selected, and three studies did not include all races. Four studies included the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised (PPVT-R) as a dependent variable, and four studies used the Behavior Problems Index (BPI). Vandell and Ramanan (1992) conducted the only study that examined school achievement. There was surprisingly little overlap across studies in the construction of maternal employment variables. There was also considerable variation in the background variables used as controls. The number of control variables varied from 4 to over 25. Mother's IQ, race, and family income or poverty status were the only variables that were controlled across all six studies. Child gender was the only moderating variable examined in all six studies. Only one study directly evaluated potential mediators of early maternal employment. It is therefore less surprising that these studies yielded very different findings. In general, the Vandell and Ramanan (1992) , Parcel and Menaghan (1994) , and Greenstein (1995) studies found no adverse effects of early maternal employment on PPVT-R or behavior problems, and Vandell and Ramanan (1992) found some positive effects on children's Peabody Individual Achievement Test (PIAT) scores. Desai, Chase-Lansdale, and Michael (1989) found negative effects on PPVT-R scores only for boys from high-income families, and Bayder and Brooks-Gunn (1991) found negative effects on PPVT-R and behavior problems for White families only. Belsky and Eggebeen (1991) found adverse effects on their variable called ADJUST, a composite of BPI scores and temperament variables, but when they examined BPI scores alone, they did not find significant effects. Because the studies were based on the same data set, the conflicting results must be due to one or more of the methodological variations, although it is not clear which. Possible ways in which these methodological differences could have caused these discrepancies and affected results are explored next. An Examination of Various Methodological Approaches and Their Possible Effects on Findings Sample selection. Differences in sample selection might be one possible reason for the discrepancies. The children studied by Vandell and Ramanan (1992) were much older and their mothers were much younger than in any other study. Thus, it is possible that any negative effects of early maternal employment disappear by second grade or that maternal employment is not harmful for younger, lower income mothers. Similarly, as Bayder and Brooks-Gunn (1991) pointed out, the discrepancy between their results and those reported by Desai et al. (1989) may have been because the former study included only White children. There are theoretical reasons why the effects of early maternal employment might vary depending on the age of the child. The effects might fade over time as more proximal factors play a larger role in a child's development. On the other hand, sleeper effects might be observed as minor, early negative effects spiral into larger problems. A cross-study comparison of these six studies supports the former hypothesis. However, a within-study comparison of children across a wide age range would better address this issue. This was not feasible using the 1986 NLSY but is an option with the more recent version. Each of the studies recognized the importance of addressing race but did so in different ways. Some limited their sample to certain racial groups, arguing that other racial groups were not adequately represented and that the assessments were conducted in English. Others included all children but controlled for race. Using the former approach limits the generalizability of the results, whereas using the latter approach may mask important racial differences. Greenstein (1995) included all races and performed separate analyses by race. This avoids both of these problems, but it does not directly test whether race moderates the effects of early employment, and it increases the number of analyses and the chance of Type I error. A modification of the approach used by Vandell and Ramanan (1992) would avoid these problems: include all races and evaluate the moderating effect of race. In the updated NLSY, adequate sample size for each racial group is no longer an issue. In addition, beginning in 1988 the PPVT-R was administered in Spanish to those children who so preferred, decreasing somewhat the cultural bias of the assessment. It is important to note that results would still need to be interpreted with caution given questions regarding the validity of standardized cognitive assessments in non-White children. Constructing maternal employment variables. The different approaches to maternal employment variable construction could be another reason for the discrepancies. For example, Parcel and Menaghan (1994) , who found no effects, examined the number of hours worked among employed mothers, whereas each of the other studies involved comparisons between employed mothers and nonemployed mothers. It may be that employment status rather than the intensity of employment is related to child development. Consistent with this hypothesis, Desai et al. (1989) found no effects on PPVT-R scores when they compared mothers working full time during the first year with mothers who did not, whereas Bayder and Brooks-Gunn (1991) found an effect on PPVT-R scores when they compared mothers who worked at least 1 hr per week during the first year with mothers who did not work during the first year. In each study, the primary approach to constructing maternal employment variables was to create categories of early employment characteristics and dummy-code variables to examine each category. Creating categorical maternal employment variables can be useful in interpreting results because it is sometimes easier to use categories to organize information. Researchers have also justified using categories to detect nonlinear effects ( Parcel & Menaghan, 1994 ). However, this approach has been criticized ( Scarr, 1991 ) and presents the following problems. For the most part, these categories were formed from continuous variables, so there are infinite ways one could form categories. This approach can also exclude some participants if the categories do not include every employment pattern. For example, Belsky and Eggebeen's (1991) approach to variable construction excluded almost half of the children in their study. This approach also results in arbitrary boundaries between categories. Participants with similar employment patterns will often be assigned to two different categories. Furthermore, using continuous variables does not preclude one from examining nonlinear effects of early employment and in fact provides more sensitivity in detecting such relationships. Another problem with previous studies' use of categories is that they often combined different dimensions. For example, comparing mothers who worked full time during the first year with mothers who did not combines information about the timing and intensity of early employment. It is not clear whether results reflect one or both employment dimension. Selecting control variables. The six studies varied in their approaches to selection of control variables, with some studies controlling for many more variables than others did. Some studies used theory to guide variable selection, others used empirical methods, and others used a combination. In addition, some studies controlled for family variables assessed at the same time that the dependent variables were measured (in 1986), whereas others did not. Three of the studies that used theory only used a larger number of control variables and tended to control for 1986 variables. These three studies were also the only ones that failed to find any adverse effects of maternal employment. Controlling for relevant background family characteristics reduces third-variable effects and removes error from the dependent variable. However, if a background variable does not correlate with child outcome, then it cannot act as a third variable and including it will not remove error. In fact, including irrelevant variables in regression models will on average increase standard errors and make it more likely that Type II error will occur. Therefore, the three studies may have failed to find adverse effects because they included irrelevant variables. Thus, in addition to using theory in control variable selection, it is important to empirically evaluate whether each background variable should be included in the regression equations. In selecting control variables, one must distinguish between selection factors and mediators of the effect of early employment on children. Selection factors are variables that might affect, but are not affected by, early employment. These might include parents' education and mothers' IQ. Mediators are variables that are affected by patterns of early employment and in turn affect children. Thus, many variables assessed in 1986 should not be considered selection factors. Selection factors and mediating effects must be examined using different analytic approaches. For example, each of the six studies controlled for family income or poverty status measured after the birth of the child. One of the ways in which early maternal employment may positively affect children may be through its financial benefits. Controlling for income after the child is born involves controlling for an important benefit of maternal employment. It is not surprising, then, that some of these studies found detrimental effects of maternal employment when they statistically removed its positive effects. Family income before the birth of the child should be controlled, because the wealth of the family is likely to affect whether a mother chooses to be employed. However, income after the child's birth should be examined as a mediating variable of positive effects of early employment. Improvements in the Sample The sample available in the more recent NLSY is stronger than the 1986 sample used by five of the six studies in several ways. First, the 1986 NLSY data on which these studies were based were from unusually young, low-socioeconomic status (SES) mothers. The NLSY has continued to follow these women and their children, and the data set now contains more older, higher SES mothers. Thus, the most recent version of the NLSY allows for an examination of the effects of early maternal employment in a more representative sample. Whereas the children in the 1986 data were estimated to represent the first 40% of the offspring that these women would bear, the recent data set is estimated to represent the first 70% to 75% of these women's offspring ( Center for Human Resource Research, 1997 ). In addition, the sample size is now much larger. This should allow for more stable, robust results and should provide more confidence that null effects truly represent no or negligible effects rather than Type II error. Furthermore, the more recent data allow for an examination of much longer term effects, because there are now a sizable number of elementary school age children in the sample. This also means that two additional outcome variables specific to older children are available: self-esteem and academic achievement. Paternal Employment In contrast to the focus on the effects of maternal employment on child development, few studies have examined early paternal employment. McHale and Huston (1984) found that fathers' time in paid work affected the amount but not the quality of interaction with their infants. However, Parcel and Menaghan's (1994) analysis of the NLSY indicated that fathers working fewer hours during their children's first few years was associated with more behavior problems in children. The small body of literature on fathers' employment focuses on job quality or unemployment rather than on how the time demands of fathers' jobs affect the family (e.g., Barling, 1986 ). The Present Study The more recent NLSY provides the opportunity to reexamine the effects of early parental employment on children's development, addressing a number of important limitations of these six previous studies as follows: (a) A larger, more representative sample was used that should significantly increase the generalizability of results; (b) selection factors were distinguished from mediating variables; (c) control variables were selected using both theory and empirical decision rules; (d) the sample contained a wider age range of children allowing for an evaluation of short-term and long-term effects of early employment; (e) children from all races were included; and (f) whenever possible, continuous variables of early parental employment were used rather than artificially creating categorical variables. Thus, by drawing on the methodological strengths and addressing the methodological weaknesses of previous studies of early parental employment, it is hoped that this study will more fully realize the potential of the NLSY to address this topic. Past research and theory have been conflicting regarding the effects of early parental employment, making it difficult to make specific predictions regarding the presence or direction of effects. Although previous inconsistent results are likely caused by methodological differences, the studies differ in too many ways to determine which findings are correct. Thus, the goal of this study is to examine whether and when early parental employment affects children's emotional, cognitive, and academic development. Method The NLSY Sample The NLSY is a survey of approximately 12,600 individuals who have been interviewed annually since 1979 when they were 14 to 22 years old. This survey oversampled African American, Hispanic, and economically disadvantaged White individuals. The economically disadvantaged White oversample was dropped in 1990 for financial reasons. In 1986, 1988, 1990, 1992, and 1994, the survey conducted child assessments on offspring of the female participants. Children of all races who were between 3 and 12 years of age at any of the five child assessment dates and who were born in 1980 or later (several background variables were unavailable for children born before 1980) were included. To assess possible developmental differences in the effects of early employment, I examined four different age groups separately: 3- to 4-year-olds, 5- to 6-year-olds, 7- to 9-year-olds, and 10- to 12-year-olds. See Table 1 for descriptive statistics on this sample. It should be noted that there is partial overlap of participants across these four age groups; each age group contains some participants who were also included in another age group. Of the 4,924 children who were 3- to 4-year-olds, 3,371 were also in the 5- to 6-year-old group. There were 4,486 children in the 5- to 6-year-old group, 3,203 of whom were also in the 7- to 9-year-old group. There were 3,711 children in the 7- to 9-year-old group, 1,951 of whom were also in the 10- to 12-year-old group. There were 2,095 children in the 10- to 12-year-old group. Thus, some of the data in this study are longitudinal and some are cross-sectional. (These figures are based on the number of children in each age group who had scores on at least one of the child outcome measures. The actual number of participants for each analysis varies because not all children were administered all measures.) In addition, although this sample is more representative than samples used in previous studies of early maternal employment ( Bayder & Brooks-Gunn, 1991 ; Belsky & Eggebeen, 1991 ), it still does not represent women who bear children after age 34 and is still somewhat socioeconomically disadvantaged. Measurement of Variables Early maternal employment variables. Five indexes of early maternal employment were used. They were constructed on the basis of mothers' reports of how many weeks after their children's birth they returned to work and their estimates of the average number of hours they worked per week during each quarter-year of the first 3 years of their children's lives. For an evaluation of whether early employment status affects children, the first variable indicated whether or not the mother was employed at some time during the child's first 3 years ( employment during the first 3 years ); mothers who reported returning to work by the 156th week after their child's birth were coded 1 and mothers who did not return to work in the first 3 years were coded 0. The second, third, and fourth variables were applicable only for women who were employed at some point during the first 3 years. The second variable was the number of weeks after the child's birth before the mother returned to work ( timing of early employment ), the third variable was the average number of hours per week that she worked when she returned to work ( early employment hours ), and the fourth variable was the number of quarters the mother did not work after she had returned to work ( discontinuous employment ). Early employment hours was constructed by identifying the quarter-year during which the mother returned to work and calculating the average number of hours per week she worked from this quarter-year until the child's third birthday (not including quarters during which she reported working 0 hr). Many of the previous studies on the NLSY have included a variable representing whether or not mothers worked during the first year of the child's life. Although the timing of early maternal employment should capture the effects of employment during the first year, a variable employment during the first year was also included to facilitate comparison of the results of this study with the findings of previous studies. Note that employment during the first 3 years and early employment hours could have been combined into one variable by assigning mothers who were never employed during the first 3 years a score of 0 hr. However, doing so assumes that the difference between not being employed and being employed 5 hr per week is the same as the difference between working 35 hr per week and working 40 hr per week. That is, it could not be assumed that not being employed is simply one end of the employment intensity dimension. Early employment status (comparing being employed with not being employed) might have a different effect on children than the intensity of employment and was therefore examined separately. Early parental employment variable. Mothers reported the average number of hours per week that their spouses spent at their jobs every year from 1979 to 1994. The variable for early paternal employment hours consisted of an average of the spouse's job hours over the first 3 yearly assessments following the birth of the child. Child outcome measures. Five child outcome variables were examined: compliance, behavior problems, cognitive development, self-esteem, and academic achievement. For children who were in one of the four specified age ranges at more than one assessment date, their multiple scores were averaged to obtain the best possible estimate of their functioning. Compliance was assessed using a six-item subscale from the Temperament scale, which was developed for the NLSY. This scale correlates modestly but significantly with later behavior problems ( Center for Human Resource Research, 1995 ). Higher scores indicate greater compliance. Children's behavior problems were assessed using the BPI, which was developed to measure behavior problems in children age 4 and older. Many items were derived from the Child Behavior Checklist ( Achenbach & Edelbrock, 1981 ) and other child behavior scales ( Graham & Rutter, 1968 ; Kellam, Branch, Agrawal, & Ensminger, 1975 ; Peterson & Zill, 1986 ; Rutter, 1970 ). The parent reports the frequency with which the child exhibited each of 28 specific problems (1 = often true, 2 = sometimes true, 3 = not true ). Standard scores (based on all children, not same sex) were used, with higher scores indicating more behavior problems. This scale has demonstrated good construct validity ( Center for Human Resource Research, 1993 ). Children's self-esteem was measured using the global self-worth subscale of the Self-Perception Profile for Children. This is a self-report measure that assesses children's sense of self-competence in the domain of academic skills and general self-worth ( Harter, 1985 ). It correlates highly with teacher ratings and has good reliability ( Harter, 1985 ). This measure was administered to children age 8 years and older. Children's cognitive development was assessed using the PPVT-R ( Dunn & Dunn, 1981 ), a widely used test of receptive language. This measure was administered to all children age 3 years and older. It was administered to all children in 1986 and 1992, and only to children without a previous valid score in 1988, 1990, and 1994. Children's academic achievement was measured using the PIAT. Three subtests from the PIAT were administered to children age 5 years and older: mathematics, reading recognition, and reading comprehension. Standard scores were used for both the PPVT-R and PIAT. Both cognitive measures have good reliability and validity ( Dunn & Dunn, 1981 ; Dunn & Markwardt, 1970 ). Selection factors. Theory and previous research suggested a number of family background variables that might act as selection factors for early employment. The following background variables were created for examining the effects of early maternal employment: family income, mother's education, mother's age at the child's birth, child gender, mother's IQ, child's race, birth order of the child, and marital status. For the effects of parental employment, the following background characteristics were used: family income, father's education, father's age at the child's birth, child's gender, child's race, and birth order of the child. Fathers' IQs were not assessed in the NLSY. Mother's reports of these variables at the first assessment following the birth of the child were used, except for income, which was based on reports regarding the year before the child's birth. Mother's intelligence was assessed using the Armed Forces Qualification Test (AFQT), which was administered to all NLSY participants in 1980. The AFQT consists of the sum of scores on four subtests of the Armed Services Vocational Aptitude Battery: word knowledge, numeric operations, paragraph comprehension, and arithmetic reasoning. Moderating variables. On the basis of theory and research, the following variables were examined as possible moderators of the effects of early parental employment: marital status, race, gender, family income, and job satisfaction. The six NLSY studies yielded conflicting findings regarding the moderating effects of these variables, but each variable was supported by at least one study. Job satisfaction was assessed by asking individuals to indicate how they felt about their current jobs on a 4-point scale from 1 = like it very much to 4 = dislike it very much. Previous st 1